Epistemic status: I'm diagnosed with autism. High risk of overgeneralising from my own experience to autists in general.
Intense world theory is the best explanation for Autism I've heard of. Thanks for spreading the word.
But I feel like this blogpost don't explain it as well as the book, even though you quote the book. I think this is probaby because the sections you qoted are the ones focused on explaining why autists have worse social skills, and I have come to belive that having bad social skills are not very stongly linked to autism, at least in the narow sense of understaning nonverbal comuniction.
Sure many atusits have social dificulties. But I don't think that's because we have trouble reading faces or anything like that. I'm not saying I'm an expert, but have you noticed how bad most neurtypicals are correctly reading faces and bodylanguage? It's mostly guesswork and overconfidence.
Autists run into socail problem becaue we act diffrently, (e.g. lack or eye contact) which makes the neurotypical nerouvs, which makes interaction harder. A lot of social interaction is about getting the other person to feel at ease in your company. Getting over stimulated and having a meltdown is also extremly bad for ones social acceptance (understandingly). But neither of these dificulties comes primarlely from lack of social skills.
I know I'm supposed to say things like "I'm fine" even when that is a lie. I know the rules as well as anyone. It's not that complicated. But lying hurts! Most neurotypicals can not grok this simple fact, which leads to some rediculus theories about autism.
This is relatable. I'm was diagnosed ASPD as a child, but never had any follow-up treatment or therapy. One noteworthy aspect of my transition into adulthood is the sheer amount of deliberate practice that went into learning how to properly socialize. Standing on the other side of all that effort, I feel that I've become more empathic than half my neurotypical friends and family members, quicker to accurately and elaborately imagine (in a humanizing fashion) another person's perspective. People regard me as eloquent and charming to be around, confident and outgoing, etc. Honestly, it's just a lot of hard work. My tendency is toward seclusion, I'm strongly introverted, and social gatherings of more than, say, three-to-five close friends can easily spike my blood pressure.
When it comes to certain things, I'm definitely having a qualitatively different experience than some of my more neurotypical friends. You brought up lying and that's one good example. I think that a common manifestation of neurodivergence is an obsession with the true shape of things, so that it's easy to become hyperfocused, to a detriment, on distinctions that seem very important to the individual.
I think neurotypicals' guesses are more accurate; that's, IMO, the mechanism by which empathy works, a propensity to epistemically-unjustified intuitions which are in fact true for most other people.
Neurotypicals are more accurate for other neurotypicals. Autists are more accurate for other autists.
Since there are more neurotypicals (are there? or just more people pretending to be?*) neurotypicals are statistically more often correct**. But I would still not claim that that is a higher level of social skill. Having higher skill level at a more common task, is not the same thing as having higher over all skill. This detail is very important for understanding autism.
* Not saying that autism is the majority. But there are more neurotypes out there, and probably lots that are better at masking than autists.
** In a statistically representative environment. When autists are free to self segregate, we no longer have problems. This is the main point actually. If it was just a one dimensional skill issue, concentrating lots of autists, would go terribly, since no one have social skills, but instead is's great. All the people I get along with the best are other autists (officially or self diagnosed).
that's, IMO, the mechanism by which empathy works
Empathy is a very unreliable source of information though. E.g. I feel empathy with my plushies.
Empathy is a useful tool, I use it too to generate initial guesses about people. But I'm also aware that it's untrustworthy. In my experience it's common for neurotypicals to fail at this last step.
Autists are more accurate for other autists than they are for neurotypicals, but they are worse than neurotypicals at understanding neurotypicals and only about equal to them at understanding fellow autists, which is IMO reflective of a lower baseline aptitude somewhat compensated for by shared ground.
I agree that empathy makes one irrational about inanimate objects; I think this is because the most useful level of empathy for predicting other humans also produces many false positives when applied to things that are not other humans.
Actually, it's probably true if you don't confound for intelligence.
Autism is negatively correlated with intelligence, and if you're not very smart, everything gets harder. But I think it's wrong to see low intelligence as part of Autism. And even if you disagree, it's weird to classify a general intelligence problem as a specific social deficit problem.
But if you compare high functioning Autists with neurotypicals, in realistic enough settings, I'm convinced autists will be better at understanding autists than neurotypicals are at understanding autists. Although "realistic enough" might require the autists enough time to interact to spot each other as same type of person.
I don't put a lot of weight here on academic studies, over just all of my life experience. But in case you do: I did here of a study where autist worked better with other autists than neurotypicals with neurotypicals. I don't have the liks, sorry. Just my memory of someone I trust telling me about it.
The reason I don't trust academic studies on this topic, is that is really really hard to do them well, so most of them are not done well.
My description is based on extensive observation and to a lesser extent personal experience, and was not intended to focus on those with low intelligence; while it's difficult to assess intelligence over the internet, I haven't especially noticed a correlation between high intelligence and increased social skill.
To bring in some concrete examples of places autists fail to impress (this list is far from complete, but hopefully at least gestures in an informative direction):
Things allists are generally better at:
-correctly inferring my mood from nonverbal cues
-executing the social graces which make me feel safe and welcome; not defecting by accident (note, ironically enough, that—while I'm sure his algorithm does play better with normies—the author of that post also demonstrated himself in the comments to be terrible at picking a tone which wouldn't insult lesswrongers; this IMO ties into a common autistic tendency to have difficulty with code-switching and with real-time adjustment to social feedback)
-understanding the gist of what I said when I didn't use precisely the words they consider correct; understanding what I say when I happen to use imprecise words because my desired level of abstraction does not contain more precision
-sending positive phatic signals in the event they happen to like me (and, note, IRL I don't generally look at people when they're talking to me at all; if I did, I expect the difference would be far more pronounced due to allists' generally higher inclination to express emotion in their faces and bodies)
-both groups have a deeply unpleasant tendency to demand one spell oneself out to them in excruciating detail, but autists generally require a higher level of detail and have a lower threshold of confusion
Things they're about equal at:
-allists are likelier to impute an emotional tone to their interlocutors' words which wasn't there whilst autists are likelier to impute spurious implications, but both groups routinely fall into both failure modes
-both groups also have a strong tendency to dig their heels in if it's suggested they might've misunderstood something, arguing that their interlocutor obviously meant X and was using words wrong or is lying about their intent (autists are likelier to use the former excuse whilst the latter is more often heard from allists, but again, both groups commit both errors)
-autists have a tendency to assume that which they can't immediately see the point of is objectively irrelevant or useless, rather than their having failed to understand something; allistic self-centeredness and overconfidence does produce similar results in many cases, but doesn't usually manifest in this precise fashion
-both groups also tend to assume that if they're confused it's because their interlocutor is talking nonsense, rather than because they've failed to parse their interlocutors' words correctly and/or gather necessary context
-allists aren't discernibly worse at reading all the words I said and assuming I chose them deliberately, although this is more because autists are surprisingly bad at it than because allists are any good at it
The intense world theory sounds similar to Eysenck's theory of introversion vs extraversion from the 1960s. One summary:
Eysenck (2008/1967) believed that differences in extraversion were due to physiological differences in brain systems that caused some people to be more easily aroused than others. Specifically, he proposed that the ascending reticular activating system (ARAS) regulated arousal. People differ in the sensitivity of this system, which causes them to respond differently to their environment. A moderately stimulating environment might cause people with a very sensitive ARAS to feel overstimulated and retreat (i.e., to behave like introverts), but people with a less sensitive ARAS to feel understimulated and seek out additional stimulation (i.e., to behave like extraverts).
The inverted U arousal graph is typically plotted as Performance vs. Arousal and called the Yerkes–Dodson law.
Another summary here; more findable with keywords like "optimal level of arousal", Eysenck, and extraversion.
Great post, as always.
They determined that local neuronal networks in the three brain regions tested in rats are hyperreactive….
Now that's an experimental validation.
Further, the Markrams and their team found that neural networks in valproic acid–treated rats are also hyperplastic... When connectivity patterns between neurons in local and expanded networks were examined before and after widespread and prolonged (overnight) activation of the entire network, valproic acid–treated rats exhibited an increase in the rate of rewiring, mostly evident in the nonlocal networks, compared to controls…
If this applies to autistic humans too then it would have interesting implications.
Your sensory sensitivity chart is accurate for autistic people (acknowledging that there is variation from person to person). For me in particular, it slightly underestimates how easily I get startled.
The starting point has to be innate, not learned. We're talking here about startle reflexes, orienting reactions (e.g. where you turn your head and body towards a sound), flinching, releasing cortisol, etc. Nobody teaches you how to do those things! Moreover, there's nothing in your sensory input data that says that an unexpected 27 dB sound is sufficiently threatening to warrant startle and cortisol, but an unexpected 19 dB sound is not. This is a heuristic threshold set by the genome. No wonder it's different in different people! (It could also be modified over time by experience, I'm not sure. But anyway, it has to start somewhere.)
I always like information that helps me distinguish hard-coded from trainable attributes.
I figure what happens as a consequence, at least sometimes, is: the person with autism gradually learns to interact with and understand people, but without using empathetic simulation. Instead, they just take their general intelligence, and leverage it to build a new human model from the ground up, just as people can model any complicated system from the ground up (e.g. a complicated piece of software). So there's still a human model, but it's built on a different foundation—a foundation without that strong innate connection to brainstem circuits.
I think this explains why (IIUC) there are intelligent people with autism who are able to do theory-of-mind-type reasoning (as in the book excerpt at the top), and able to understand social interactions and conventions, but need to deliberately learn aspects of these things that neurotypical people might find intuitive and effortless.
I can confirm this happened for at least . I built a model of human beings from first principles by systematically combing through the scientific, historical and economic literature.
Maybe people with schizophrenia have their innate social instincts very powerfully active all the time, for whatever reason.
When I interact with fortune tellers, I feel like their social intuitive sense is supercharged in a way that's very similar to reports of schizophrenia. The difference is that healthy fortune tellers can tell that their intuitions in their head are not "real" in the physical sense whereas schizophrenics can't tell the difference. (It gets confusing because fortune tellers frequently do believe in literal magic [which is not real] but there's a big difference between being a Wiccan and being a schizophrenic.)
I can confirm this happened for at least . I built a model of human beings from first principles by systematically combing through the scientific, historical and economic literature.
Me too. Although my soruces where mostly asking my parents and other kind people to explain. Often they where not able to, but over time I got enough pices.
lsusr, do you find empatic simulations to be painfull? Because I don't. When I'm able to look at someone and feel what they are feeling, that's awsome.
But most of the time this don't work, because
a) Neruotypicals are too alien. Although I'm getting better, by conecting my deliberatly learned model to my empathy model. It's similar the colurblind box in Example 1 one here, but much more extensive.
b) I have high epistemic standards. Sort of the oposite of what you expereince with the fortuen teller. My empathy needs a stong evidence to trigger.
I'm not a neuroscientist but I think a nice corollary to your theory would be to look into the minds of those with addictions. I've read a lot about addictions online and talked to a few addicts who are addicted to different things (Alcohol, narcotics, etc). They all seem to have induced a dim world within themselves, but a specific one that is usually only acted out upon with their drug of choice, rather than like a psychopathic child killing animals. But late stage addicts do routinely do socially harmful behavior. All addicts seem to have a similar ratchet effect take place where they get their high, the high becomes their baseline, the rest of the world goes dim or less stimulating by default and to get a new high they have to go to a more potent drug / stimulating resource.
I also noticed they usually seem to hide their warped utility function from themselves, only addressing it when it takes over their entire life (rock bottom). Which I think might be an interesting way to look at alignment problems because addicts are basically misaligned mesa-optimized humans that has their "inner process" hiding their warped true reward function from themselves. From the point of view of addiction recovery it's also interesting to see how a misaligned person tries to change their values to something that produces better long term outcomes for that person, to varying degrees of success.
I don't know if looking into addictions would be something your interested in, but I figured it was worth bringing up when I read your dim world theory.
Hyporesponse to social stimuli can be explained in terms of the emotional intensity of the signal...
Also: attempting to model another person's mind works much less well if the other person's mind works much differently. (This contrasts with your model in which autistic poeple avoid using empathetic simulation. I propose they use it and it produces poor predictions, but we could both be right.)
Personally I'm overwhelmed by some social signals (especially negative ones, which feel dramatically more intense than positive ones, easily to the point of disrupting my life), and I ambiguously fit the criteria for Asperger's. So I'm curious what surveys of autistic patients, and autopsies, have been done to check this theory.
My (weak!) understanding of psychopathy was that their theory of mind is pretty much fine except that the "empathy" part of it fails in some way. In my model (1st paragraph), if psychopathy were a matter of underconnection, psychopaths should have mispredictions when simulating other people, but they have a reputation for charisma, charm and backstabbing, suggesting high social competence but low empathy. Indeed, low empathy (by itself) might even improve the ability to simulate because higher brain functions that want to do analysis are less interrupted by emotion. (Perhaps a high-functioning psychopath has reduced empathy, but not reduced to zero: enough empathy to predict other people, without the irritation of intensity of feeling.)
My model seems to predict that it should be possible to be both autistic and schizophrenic, whereas it shouldn't be possible to be psychopathic and autistic. As far as I can tell, both those are basically right.
What's the evidence for this?
Also: attempting to model another person's mind works much less well if the other person's mind works much differently.
This is known as the Double Empathy Problem and has been described by Milton (2012) in On the ontological status of autism: the ‘double empathy problem’. Some excerpts:
In a sense it is a 'double problem' as both people experience it, and so it is not a singular problem located in any one person. Rather, it is based in the social interaction between two differently disposed social actors, the disjuncture being more severe for the non-autistic disposition as it is experienced as unusual, while for the ‘autistic person’ it is a common experience.
In analysing the interactions that ‘autistic people’ have with the wider population, it is easy to problematise the definition of autism as a ‘social deficit’ located within an individual’s mind. Differences in neurology may well produce differences in sociality, but not a ‘social deficit’ as compared to an idealised normative view of social reality. Such definitions may help to signpost disability support services, but they are no way of defining autism in any kind of holistic sense.
I want to distinguish two stories about empathy:
(1) (conventional story) The point of empathy is to form factually/predictively accurate beliefs about another person's state of mind and therefore what they will do and feel
(2) (my idiosyncratic story) The point of empathy is that, if you're talking to someone or thinking about someone, and you are aware of how they're feeling, that "awareness" (in neurotypical brains) takes the form of a transient automatic empathetic simulation of the other person, and this mechanism causes automatic "response feelings" in oneself—and these "response feelings" might be pro-social and pro-understanding, but also might not.
For example, if I think someone is angry at me, I might automatically get angry back. If I think someone is feeling condescending towards me, I might automatically get defensive and angry. If I think someone is sad, I might automatically feel sympathetic towards them (or feel triumphant over them! These are complicated circuits!) Etc. etc.
I was talking about (2). My theory is that in autism the "response feelings" in story (2) are so strong that they're aversive, and people with classic autism adopt the coping strategy of avoiding invoking them altogether, by avoiding the specific type of mental operation that I call "empathetic simulation". And in psychopaths, the "response feelings" are so weak as to be barely felt, and psychopaths adopt the coping strategy of seeking out situations that invoke unusually strong "response feelings", like watching people suffer.
As for (1), I do not in fact believe that the neurotypical suite of social instincts (including the transient effortless type of "empathetic simulation") are particularly effective at forming factually/predictively accurate beliefs about another person's state of mind.
(Example neurotypical conversation:
--"Hey Emily, you got mud all over the refrigerator again, c'mon you gotta clean that up!"
--"God, Mom, you're always talking about the fridge, it's like you're totally obsessed with it, we both know you don't REALLY care about the fridge, you're REALLY just pissed off that I found a boyfriend who loves me, and you just want me to be miserable and lonely like you are!"
…Well, not a lot of factually accurate beliefs about mind-states going on in this conversation!)
So in terms of (1), I would strongly agree with your proposal of "attempting to model another person's mind works much less well if the other person's mind works much differently". I would also add something like "all the ways that neurotypical people form factually inaccurate beliefs about other people's minds get treated as 'normal human relations', but all the ways that people with autism form factually inaccurate beliefs about other people's minds get treated as pathological." Some combination of those two things, I figure.
What's the evidence for this?
Umm, maybe 30 minutes of google searching :-P I'm open to being corrected obviously
There's a distinction that is important to make when it comes to empathy, between 'cognitive empathy' and 'emotional empathy', that is a good starting place. The empathy divide goes all ways when it comes to neurology - neurotypicals have a harder time understanding autistics and (the opposite neurotype of autistic folk, who might have been diagnosed psychopaths at some point but don't have a definite entry in the DSM-V).
Cognitive empathy is being able to understand another person's perspective and mental state. Everyone needs to learn this, but if people think similarly to you it is easier because you have some baseline assumptions that are correct. It is often more urgent to learn this for people who don't share your neurotype if you are autistic (or the specific neurotype that is psychopathic?-opposite-of-autism) because you aren't in the majority in most circumstances - and assuming that everyone thinks the same way you do leads to wrong conclusions that cause wrong predictions quite quickly.
Affective or emotional empathy. Responding to another's mental state with our own emotions, when we are affected by them. Sympathy, concern, and personal distress tend to fall into this. We vary as much as anybody on this one, with some potential complications - we may be hyperempathic, and emotionally respond more than neurotypicals to a wider range of stimuli, including perceiving impossible things like the emotional state of a toaster due to hypersensitivity. We may be alexithymic, and not understand our own emotions enough to be able to express them at all without some aggressive introspection, much less appropriately. We may be too overwhelmed by the emotion to use it to find an appropriate response (try literally feeling your pain under intense world theory). We may not know any appropriate response, or the generally appropriate response for our neurotype is not the appropriate one for yours (whereas it may have been a good guess with minds more like ours). If we do know an appropriate response, expressing ourselves with a script, with nonverbal communication, or any other 'odd' way may be more difficult to understand or receive on their end.
Is it possible to make the world "dimmer" by gradually exposing yourself to stronger and stronger stimuli?
I have a little bit of positive experience with reducing my sensitivity to loud ambulance sirens in my street. The exercise was very simple and I have a vague idea of the theory behind it.
The theory goes like this: In biology/psychology there's a distinction between different reactions to sudden and intense sensory stimuli. An orientation reflex on the one hand and a startle (and/or defense?) reflex on the other hand. The difference that's relevant here is that the orientation reflex quickly habituates while the startling does not. For instance, maybe you have a new washing machine that makes a strange, unfamiliar sound. For many people this sound may catch their attention, they double-check where it is coming from (orientation), then they quickly habituate, and after a handful of times they don't even hear the sound anymore. On the other hand, a sound may be be startling you every time and you tense up and there's no habituation no matter how often you hear the sound. Orientation reflex vs startling reflex, respectively.
Is there a way to facilitate an orientation reflex (including habituation) to kick in instead of a startle reflex? Given that these reflexes are not under under conscious control?
The exercise suggested that you consciously move your head and even the upper body (I think - or was it the whole body?) towards the source of the sudden noise and look in its direction. Even if you already know what it was and where it came from. Even if you know that there's nothing to see there because you can't see the street from there, for instance. Just move the body as if it had a proper orientation reflex. My understanding is, that this lowers the threshold for the orientation reflex to really kick in. For me at least it worked. I could usually note that there was an actual shift in the autonomic nervous system by having to yawn or taking a deep breath. And my sensitivity to the noises significantly reduced after a few weeks.
Unfortunately, I don't remember a good reference for the exercise. Nor am I well-versed with respect to those reflexes. Does anyone know more about it?
Hmm, I was writing recently about how David Burns makes exposure therapy sound like a miracle cure for all sorts of things, including some things that I wouldn't have previously expected to be treatable by exposure therapy (and he says that exposure therapy doesn't have to look like the classic "graduated exposure"). But he doesn't say anything about sensory sensitivity or autism in the book.
So I dunno. I guess it doesn't sound totally crazy off the top of my head, but that's just casual speculation, from someone who is an expert on neither exposure therapy nor autism. :-)
This link seems to be "we tried exposure therapy for sound sensitivity, and it didn't work", but I would be more inclined to describe it as "this isn't proper exposure therapy at all, it's just making loud noises in kids' ears for a minute or two". This seems relevant but I don't know how to find the associated paper if any. This and this makes it sound like exposure therapy for sensory sensitivity is already routinely used for people with autism, at least to some extent, and it works like normal.
Many years ago I read this NYTimes article (paywalled, DM or email me for a PDF) about Applied Behavior Analysis (note: see Ann Brown's reply to this comment), claiming it could "cure" classic autism; if that's true (a big "if"!) I guess we should consider the hypothesis that the underlying mechanism is "basically just lots and lots of exposure therapy for everything, day after day for years".
Many autistic individuals who experienced it have attested that Applied Behavior Analysis is an extremely unpleasant experience that caused them harm. It is a form of operant conditioning, that trains us to behave neurotypically and not behave autistically through reward/punishment. It does not 'cure' but control.
Isn't there also an intense world theory of bipolar/epilepsy? (given that both are often associated with excess neuron firing rates [or an excess of excitatory to inhibitory input], which may have some dynamics familiar to autistic brains with an excess of synaptic input]?
also related - https://opentheory.net/2023/05/autism-as-a-disorder-of-dimensionality/
https://arxiv.org/abs/1803.03635
I believe autism is a risk factor for epilepsy and bipolar, and vice-versa. But I don’t think I would describe the latter two as having “an intense world theory”. Epilepsy just means you get seizures; bipolar is when the brain dopamine reward system declares “every thought is great” for a while (mania) and then “every thought is terrible” for a longer while (depression), and repeat, or at least that’s my working hypothesis. Sure, it kinda intuitively makes sense that these three conditions might all share risk factors (well, definitely epilepsy + autism, actually the autism+bipolar connection is not immediately intuitive to me, I dunno), like maybe let’s say “trigger-happy neurons”. But I don’t think “trigger-happy neurons” is what I (or other people) mean when we say “intense world theory”. Trigger-happy neurons is an implementation-level thing, whereas “intense world theory” is a way to think about what’s happening at the algorithm-level. This is an important difference because presumably there are a variety of implementation-level things (a.k.a. root causes) that all result in similar / overlapping algorithm-level changes. And in particular, I strongly suspect that you can get autism without trigger-happy neurons—for example, it seems at least plausible to me that you could get autism from a certain kind of cerebellar abnormality even leaving the rest of the brain completely untouched. And if you do, I claim that intense-world theory is still a useful way to think about what’s happening.
Michael’s essay doesn’t really speak to me but thanks for the link anyway.
The part about hyperparameters being 'tuned' to sensory input, and how that interacts hyperplasticity, is very interesting to me. To pull that thread, I think that there's a mechanism of attention that may be important to include, based on my own experience. The theory of monotropism accounts for this, might be worth looking into?
I am autistic and hypersensitive. It has always struck people as odd that I can manage social situations quite easily as long as I calm my nervous system with additional input (body-focused repetitive behavior aka stimming). But it's not that strange - Neurotypical people stim too, chewing a pencil, pacing while deep in thought is common. Many autistic people need to move, pull, push, twirl, hum, sing, just to stay at baseline. Otherwise the world is just too much.
If autism is only hypersensitivity then this doesn't make sense, more input would be worse, right? I think the mechanism might be something like 'balancing' the sensory input and output, but I would love to find a more elegant explanation for why it 'cancels out'. I'm throwing out there that may be a unique adaptation in the hyperparameters, or whatever attention system responsible for processing inputs, to explain why stimming is so effective? I will have to think on it more.
'Heavy work' is another example. It's amazing for children and adults with autism, as a child my parents found I would be abnormally calm after pushing around heavy objects (I got a tire as a present once haha). There is something about deep pressure and vestibular input that is healing, though the effects only last up to a few days. Swinging in a hammock, weighted blankets, scuba diving, weightlifting/calisthenics, rock climbing, hiking, are all activities that autistic people I meet seem to gravitate to independently and use to regulate.
My autistic child used to be terrified of the lawn mower, even if he was inside. We couldn't use the mixer, the vacuum, or even the shower without him freaking out.
He went from terror to thinking these things were cool: if I cut the grass, he comes out to play nearby with his toy mower; he loves to vacuum. And -- glory of glories -- the shower is boring.
So I think for him at least, it's a progression from WAY TOO MUCH to fascinating and fun to bo-ring.
This leads me to wonder: if it's possible for a stimulus to be overwhelming but not too overwhelming, if exposure therapy might help rather than just making him freak. It's worth a try.
Oops, just thought of this: he loves slurpy noises near his ears. Shouldn't that be way too stimulating? It would be for me or anyone else I know! Seems autism is both about avoiding/muting stimuli and seeking them out.
if it's possible for a stimulus to be overwhelming but not too overwhelming, if exposure therapy might help rather than just making him freak. It's worth a try.
I was chatting about exposure therapy in this other comment. I wonder if we should say that everyday life can sometimes provide "unintentional exposure therapy", in which case it sounds like "exposure therapy" (in that broader sense) has already been working out for y'all, which would be really neat and interesting.
Reminder that I'm extremely not an expert and don't trust me on anything. :-)
he loves slurpy noises near his ears. Shouldn't that be way too stimulating?
Hmm, it might be an example of how that picture I drew—where valence is an inverted-U function of arousal—is a lousy model. Highly-stimulating things aren't always aversive. Like, the best and worst moments of my life were both highly-stimulating. Maybe it's like, highly-stimulating things can be very bad or very good, and not so much in between? And whether it's good or bad depends on other things going on in your brain—for example, how safe you feel. If you feel simultaneously safe and uncomfortable/scared/threatened, the two things mix together like baking soda and vinegar, and the result is…laughter! Or at least, that's what I've been figuring. To be clear, everything I'm saying here is purely coming from introspection / folk psychology, I don't have any more insight than you or anyone else.
We definitely often have both sensory-seeking and sensory-avoiding needs. I am not as sensitive to some elements of the world now as I used to be as a child, but unsure how much of that is because I was a child and now am not, compared to 'getting used to' things (or some partial loss of the actual sense rendering it moot).
Reasons it would not be worth a try are that sensory-avoided experiences are, as mentioned elsewhere, extremely painful. Literally painful, as I was surprised I had to clarify at one point - apparently people without sensory aversions can find sensory experiences metaphorically? painful without them quite registering on the same scale as getting stabbed? But I would definitely say that while I have very much not enjoyed bad ongoing physical pain, bad ongoing sensory-avoid is for my normal experiences of both a great deal worse. A migraine makes me miserable; having to endure a sufficiently unpleasant sound on an ongoing basis without recourse could have me attacking walls / trying to hit my head on things if I didn't know better / other means to try to override and distract with other sensory input.
Great article. Thank you!
I also highly commend reading the original paper referenced in the article. (full text available here https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/neuro.01.1.1.006.2007/full) Reading the original paper was quite a revelation to me. How many things are parsimoniously explained by this hypothesis.... how many things suddenly make sense. As opposed to the jumble of unrelated facts seen in most academic summaries of Autism.
There is of course opposition to the hypothesis but there has also been considerable confirmation as a look at google scholar will easily show.
This feels related to Dąbrowski's overexcitability/superstimulatability theory of giftedness. As if there are two dimensions how acutely you are experiencing stuff (how laud is the TV to use your metaphor) and how capable are you to sustain stimulation (how laud can it be for you to hear everything clearly with all the details unheard on lower volumes).
(I feel intrigued by his work on positive disintegration but haven't looked into empirical evidence yet; but my personal experience supports weaker claims about it. Actually, I don't think that it holds as strongly as the Wikipedia article presents it. But if it is directionally true, today's narrative around trauma be fairly misleading and at least unhelpful. )
This is interesting and as an autistic it suggests a new option. If this holds out then "taking a chill pill" is likely to tap into and develop those missing social capabilities. And maybe one aspect of it is triaging some of the arousal away so that the remaining functions remain operational.
One could aldso wonder whether co-occurance with depression is because the brain finds use and local optima with the passivity that comes with that. Some people might also try to do the minimum amount of autistic behaviour to make by such as keeping your gaze far enough that you are only panicing and not in terror (with maybe goal of trying to appear as neurotypical as possible). But if it is tought as overstimulation the proper responce would be to go to the most flow-like state, which would mainly be that the more you indulge the aversion the better one tends to do. So instead of being in panic, being stressed would be prerable and even further preference to be relaxed. Which in practical term would be that instead of finding social situations stressful, stop applying the one-size-fits-all shelf ready solutions and find a way to be comfortable in them even if it means your existence is a statistical outlier. You don't do better the lesser your offset is from the median.
I don't think the "on the fly" adjustement is fairly characterised as being hyperparameters. That is more like tuning regular parameters. Imagine you have car that veers to the left. You can still use it to straight if you in fact turn constantly to the right when you in fact want to go straight. The end result is very similar to no veering but your maximum turn left and maximun turn right are effected. If the situation calls for a very different balance that is far from the hyperparameters optimal then having only 5 or so levers for neurotransmitters the balance is kep int a differnt way (say that you off-set your steering being veering by your brakes being opposite veering so you constantly brake despite applying throttle)
Re: diametric models of autism and schizophrenia, I recently read a couple of posts by Vaticidalprophet on Substack which I found fairly convincing on the matter: https://vaticidalprophet.substack.com/p/williams-syndrome-syndromic-autism makes the case that the diametrical model relies on adjusting definitions when they contradict the evidence, and https://vaticidalprophet.substack.com/p/disorganized-schizotypy-and-the-problem, though mostly a critique of diagnostic measures for schizotypy, also makes the IMO decisive-if-true argument that autism and schizophrenia are positively correlated. (I've copied out the paragraph with most of his citations below for easier reference.)
Populations that are not neurotypical undergrads in Western Canada tend to, uh, not demonstrate this (see Dinsdale et al., 2013 — on which Crespi was corresponding author — for a very teeth-gritted literature review in the intro). Even in other undergrad populations Crespi’s anticorrelation is doubtful, particularly on non-SPQ instruments (Russell-Smith et al., 2011 is Dinsdale’s cite, but I’ve seen quite a few variants of this). Amongst people actually diagnosed with autism or schizotypal personality disorder, who seem like, you know, useful populations to study this in, there seems to be a there there (Barneveld et al., 2011; Esterberg et al., 2008). Outright psychosis definitely has a there there (Kiyono et al., 2020), and you keep running into hilarious studies that claim gold-standard ASD diagnostic instruments don’t work because they “misdiagnose” 30% of schizophrenics with autism (Maddox et al., 2017).5 Funniest of all, internet surveys find a large schizo-autistic population (Ford et al., 2018).
How would this theory explain the literal-mindedness and difficulty w/ non-concrete concepts commonly seen in people w/ autism? I can see how it would make effortless empathy (as in subconsciously simulating the other person) and normal social communication difficult, but the inability of many of these people on the spectrum to get metaphors or figurative speech definitely seems like something that can't be explained by "intensity" of the world around them.
Very interesting post!
1) I wonder what your thoughts are on how "disentangled" having a "dim world" perspective and being psychopathic are (completely "entangled" being: all psychopaths experience dim world and all who experience dim world are psychopathic). Maybe I'm also packing too many different ideas/connotations into the term "psychopathy".
2) Also, the variability in humans' local neuronal connection and "long-range" neuronal connections seems really interesting to me. My very unsupported, weak suspicion is that perhaps there is a correlation between these ratios (or maybe the pure # of each), and the natural ability to learn information and develop expertise in a very narrow domain of things (music, math?) vs. develop big new ideas where the concepts are largely formed from cross-domain, interdisciplinary thinking. Do you have any thoughts on this? Depending on what we believe for this, what we believe for question 1) has some very interesting implications, I think?
3) Finally, I wonder if the lesswrong community has a higher rate of "dim world" perspective-havers (or "psychopaths in the narrowly defined sense of having lower thresholds for stimulation), than the base-rate of the general population.
The first section is well-written and reliable, because I didn't write it, it's a book excerpt. The rest of the post is more prone to errors and speculations; you can skip it if you're not into that kind of stuff—I read a few books and articles about autism, but I'm definitely not an expert, and I didn't spend a lot of time checking everything I wrote. Please comment if anything seems wrong (or dumb, or offensive, etc.) so I can fix it.
Summary / Table of Contents
Introduction to the Intense World Theory of Autism
There are various theories of the root cause of autism in the brain. I'll mention a few as I go. But I'll start right in with the positive case for the theory I like: The Intense World Theory of Autism.
The Intense World Theory of Autism dates to this 2007 article. I first heard about it from the excellent book The Myth of Mirror Neurons, which devotes a whole chapter to it, excerpted below. Then I read Temple Grandin's The Autistic Brain: Thinking Across the Spectrum and found that she also brought it up, and seemed very enthusiastic about it. I've also read a couple review articles on Intense World Theory, including this one by some of the inventors of the theory, and this one and a few others. Everything I've found is pretty positive the theory—please comment if you've seen any substantive criticism.
I can't dream of describing the theory as well as Gregory Hickok does in The Myth of Mirror Neurons, so I'll just excerpt from that. First some motivation:
Then he switches to rats:
And back to humans:
There's a lot more in the book chapter, but I'm probably already pushing the limits of copyright law. I'll leave it at that. Again, this is from the book Myth of Mirror Neurons, which I heartily endorse.
Honestly, I should probably just end the blog post here. But I couldn't resist adding some more commentary and speculation, particularly trying to shift the level of discussion towards lower-level neuroscience and algorithms.
Deeper dive part 1: "Intense world"
Sensory sensitivity
Here's a chart:
I could make a similar chart for vision, touch, or any other sense. Scott Alexander brings up here the example of scratchy tags on the back of shirts, which might repeatedly draw the attention of a person with autism, yet be below the threshold where they draw the attention of most neurotypical people.
What's going on in the brain here?
The starting point has to be innate, not learned. We're talking here about startle reflexes, orienting reactions (e.g. where you turn your head and body towards a sound), flinching, releasing cortisol, etc. Nobody teaches you how to do those things! Moreover, there's nothing in your sensory input data that says that an unexpected 27 dB sound is sufficiently threatening to warrant startle and cortisol, but an unexpected 19 dB sound is not. This is a heuristic threshold set by the genome. No wonder it's different in different people! (It could also be modified over time by experience, I'm not sure. But anyway, it has to start somewhere.)
I believe that the brainstem (specifically, superior colliculus & inferior colliculus) is where you'll find these innate circuits—circuits that take a sound (or flash or touch etc.), figure out where it's coming from, and if it's sufficiently loud, execute startle reactions and orienting reactions and so on.
But that doesn't mean that these brainstem circuits are necessarily the underlying cause of sensory sensitivity in any particular person. I think other parts of the brain (amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, cerebellum) function sorta like "eager servants" of the brainstem in this task—they learn to anticipate when the brainstem is going to execute a startle, and they jump the gun, issuing those commands themselves. So if any of those systems are really trigger-happy—i.e. they issue a command in any circumstance where they think there's even the slightest chance that the brainstem would issue that command—you could get the same kind of sensory sensitivity, seems to me.
By the way, this is how I'm thinking about the (apparently) causal role of the cerebellum in autism.
Eye contact
I think eye contact is basically the same as sensory sensitivity. I believe the brainstem (superior colliculus) detects eye contact, and issues (among other things) arousal reactions ("arousal" in the psychology jargon sense, not the sexual sense—e.g. cortisol release, higher heart rate, etc.). There seems to be an arousal sweet spot: too little arousal is boring, too much arousal is overwhelming. (There are many more dimensions to your feelings than just arousal, of course! But it does seem to me that arousal is of central importance.) As in the book excerpt at the top, people with autism apparently often find that eye contact flies way past the sweet spot into "overwhelming" territory. It's aversive, and therefore they avoid it.
As in the sensory sensitivity section, the higher-than-typical arousal could come from several other sources besides the brainstem itself—particularly the amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, and cerebellum.
Social interactions more generally
I think eye-contact-detection is just one example: my hunch is that the brainstem (again, the superior & inferior colliculus) has a whole suite of heuristics for a person being near to and interacting with other people—for example, human-speech-sound-detection, and human-touch-detection, human-smell-detection, and so on. All of them create arousal, I assume, and therefore all of them might potentially create an overwhelming level of arousal in people with autism, who may correspondingly try to avoid those things.
Next up is empathetic simulation. As I discussed (speculatively) here, I think the main neurotypical way of understanding and interacting with other people is by empathetically simulating them, constantly and without any deliberate effort. (I call this "little glimpses of empathy"—to be strongly distinguished with the deliberate, slow, effortful "empathy" that people are usually talking about when they say "empathy" in everyday contexts.)
This kind of empathetic simulation is tied up with social instincts, and their corresponding arousal, which as above may be overwhelming and aversive for some people with autism.
I figure what happens as a consequence, at least sometimes, is: the person with autism gradually learns to interact with and understand people, but without using empathetic simulation. Instead, they just take their general intelligence, and leverage it to build a new human model from the ground up, just as people can model any complicated system from the ground up (e.g. a complicated piece of software). So there's still a human model, but it's built on a different foundation—a foundation without that strong innate connection to brainstem circuits.
I think this explains why (IIUC) there are intelligent people with autism who are able to do theory-of-mind-type reasoning (as in the book excerpt at the top), and able to understand social interactions and conventions, but need to deliberately learn aspects of these things that neurotypical people might find intuitive and effortless. The intuitive-and-effortless part is the pathway that goes
quick effortless empathetic simulation
→ social instincts hardwired into the brainstem
→ resulting "feeling"
…again as discussed here. (Further clarification in this comment.)
Deeper dive part 2: "Different learning algorithm hyperparameters"
I believe that a very large fraction of the human brain (96% by volume) exists for the purpose of within-lifetime learning (see discussion of “learning-from-scratch” here). They thus house learning algorithms, or more specifically “learning-and-inference algorithms”. (The "learning" part is editing synaptic connections for future use; the "inference" part is using previously-learned content for taking better actions now.)
These learning-and-inference algorithms, like any learning-and-inference algorithms, have what we call "hyperparameters" in machine learning.
Here's an example: If you've seen a pattern "A then B then C" recur 10 times in a row, you will start unconsciously expecting AB to be followed by C. But "should" you expect AB to be followed by C after seeing ABC only 2 times? Or what if you've seen the pattern ABC recur 72 times in a row, but then saw AB(not C) twice? What "should" a learning algorithm expect in those cases?
You can imagine a continuous family of learning algorithms, that operate on the same underlying principles, but have different "settings" for deciding the answer to these types of questions.
And I emphasize that this is one of many examples. "How long should the algorithm hold onto memories (other things equal)?" "How similar do two situations need to be before you reason about one by analogizing to the other?" "How much learning model capacity is allocated to each incoming signal line from the retina?" Etc. etc.
In all these cases, there is no "right" answer to the hyperparameter settings. It depends on the domain—how regular vs random are the environmental patterns you're learning? How stable are they over time? How serious are the consequences of false positives vs false negatives in different situations?
There may be an "optimal" set of hyperparameters from the perspective of "highest inclusive genetic fitness in such-and-such specific biological niche". But there is a very wide range of hyperparameters which "work", in the sense that the algorithm does in fact learn things. Different hyperparameter settings would navigate the tradeoffs discussed above—one setting is better at remembering details, another is better at generalizing, another avoids overconfidence in novel situations, another minimizes energy consumption, etc. etc.
I actually already talked about learning-and-inference algorithm hyperparameters in the "intense world" discussion of the previous section—but without using those words. Specifically I said that the brainstem has innate capability to do startle reactions, but the amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, and cerebellum all (in different ways) learn to anticipate the brainstem. And this algorithm has a tradeoff between false positives and false negatives. And this tradeoff depends on—you guessed it—hyperparameters. "Intense world" would come from the algorithms being tuned to have almost no false negatives, at the expense of lots of false positives.
In this section, I'm talking about learning-and-inference algorithm hyperparameters more generally, focusing more on memory and cognition.
I can think of two reasons that an autistic brain might wind up with systematically different learning-and-inference algorithm hyperparameters (for memory and cognition) than a neurotypical brain:
Finally, here are a couple typical aspects of autism that seem to be in the "different hyperparameters for memory and cognition" category:
Bonus: "Dim world theory of psychopathy"??
To be clear, this part is my own wild speculation. Call it the "dim world theory of psychopathy". Psychopathy, in this theory, is when the brain is hyporeactive and hypoplastic, as opposed to autism where it's hyperreactive and hyperplastic.
When people with autism activate their empathy brain circuits, it reacts so overwhelmingly that they learn early in childhood to avoid activating those circuits in the first place, using techniques like avoiding eye contact, avoiding the use of empathetic simulation as a method of understanding people, and so on.
Conversely, when people with psychopathy activate their empathy brain circuits, there's barely a whisper of activity. Why do psychopathic kids stereotypically torture animals? Because for them to feel anything at all, it takes a situation that would be emotionally overwhelming for neurotypical people.
a quiet empty room,
moving my toes
😀
🙄
boring
😡
someone, while
making eye contact
and empathetically
simulating them.
😞
😀
🙄
face as part of a
deliberate scheme to
make them
miserable
overwhelming
😱
😞
😀
I did a little search for studies about sensory sensitivity etc. in psychopaths, and didn't have much luck finding anything very informative. I didn't try very hard, maybe it's out there. (If anyone follows up, let me know what you find. (Wanna coauthor a follow-up post with me? :) ) For now, take this as casual speculation by a non-expert, as if we were chatting over drinks.
(Update 2021: here's a possible lead—this wikipedia article talks a bit about "low arousal theory" and psychopathy. The references they provide for that are a bit underwhelming though.)
(Update 2024: (1) I currently think that this section is applicable to a large subset of ASPD people, maybe the majority, including what I personally think of as the central stereotypical cases … but that there are also some people who get an ASPD diagnosis for a quite different underlying reason, namely being quick to anger. (2) I came across another potential lead for looking into “low arousal theory” here: “The best physiological indicator of which young people will become violent criminals as adults is a low resting heart rate, says Adrian Raine of the University of Pennsylvania. … Indeed, when Daniel Waschbusch, a clinical psychologist at Penn State Hershey Medical Center, gave the most severely callous and unemotional children he worked with a stimulative medication, their behavior improved”.)
Hang on, isn't schizophrenia the opposite of autism?
(Update 2024: This whole subsection is bad and should be ignored; my up-to-date opinions about schizophrenia are at Schizophrenia as a deficiency in long-range cortex-to-cortex communication (Feb 2023) and Model of psychosis, take 2 (Aug 2023). I think that in the many “diametric” cases where schizophrenia-associated gene variants are protective against autism and vice-versa, those genes are generally reducing or increasing (respectively) the quantity and strength of cortex-to-cortex communication. Other genes may be more relevant to one or the other condition, or may increase or decrease the risk of both together, in cases where the gene variant has different effects on long-range cortex-to-cortex communication (more relevant to schizophrenia) versus local connections (more relevant to autism), or if they lead to a more or less “intense world” via their effects on some other system besides the cortex (e.g. the amygdala or brainstem), etc.)
I guess there's a "diametrical" theory where autism and schizophrenia are opposites. I dunno, I know very little about schizophrenia. Here's a random thought though.
People with autism and people with psychopathy are both basically not feeling much effect of their innate social instincts most of the time as they go about their lives. It's for totally opposite reasons! And with radically different consequences! But still, this is a narrow respect in which autism and psychopathy are not opposites. So then we can talk about a different "axis" along which schizophrenia might be the opposite of autism. Maybe people with schizophrenia have their innate social instincts very powerfully active all the time, for whatever reason. Maybe those circuits just randomly trigger all the time for no reason (as opposed to the neurotypical case when they trigger at specific times like eye contact, body contact, empathetic simulation, etc.).
By the way, I don't expect that this is a complete description of schizophrenia; I would assume that (like autism) schizophrenia involves some neuron-related thing that impacts different brain systems in different ways.
My model seems to predict that it should be possible to be both autistic and schizophrenic, whereas it shouldn't be possible to be psychopathic and autistic. As far as I can tell, both those are basically right. Simon Baron-Cohen does say here that he treated a person with both autism and borderline personality disorder (BPD) (if memory serves). BPD is a bit like psychopathy, in that both involve a lack of empathy. Narcissism is in this category too. But BPD is clearly different from psychopathy. By the way, don't ask me for a theory of BPD or narcissism—I have no idea.